FAIR USE NOTICE

FAIR USE NOTICE

A BEAR MARKET ECONOMICS BLOG

OCCUPY THE SCIENTIFIC METHOD


This site may contain copyrighted material the use of which has not always been specifically authorized by the copyright owner. We are making such material available in an effort to advance understanding of environmental, political, human rights, economic, democracy, scientific, and social justice issues, etc. we believe this constitutes a ‘fair use’ of any such copyrighted material as provided for in section 107 of the US Copyright Law.

In accordance with Title 17 U.S.C. Section 107, the material on this site is distributed without profit to those who have expressed a prior interest in receiving the included information for research and educational purposes. For more information go to: http://www.law.cornell.edu/uscode/17/107.shtml

If you wish to use copyrighted material from this site for purposes of your own that go beyond ‘fair use’, you must obtain permission from the copyright owner.

FAIR USE NOTICE FAIR USE NOTICE: This page may contain copyrighted material the use of which has not been specifically authorized by the copyright owner. This website distributes this material without profit to those who have expressed a prior interest in receiving the included information for scientific, research and educational purposes. We believe this constitutes a fair use of any such copyrighted material as provided for in 17 U.S.C § 107.

Read more at: http://www.etupdates.com/fair-use-notice/#.UpzWQRL3l5M | ET. Updates
FAIR USE NOTICE FAIR USE NOTICE: This page may contain copyrighted material the use of which has not been specifically authorized by the copyright owner. This website distributes this material without profit to those who have expressed a prior interest in receiving the included information for scientific, research and educational purposes. We believe this constitutes a fair use of any such copyrighted material as provided for in 17 U.S.C § 107.

Read more at: http://www.etupdates.com/fair-use-notice/#.UpzWQRL3l5M | ET. Updates

All Blogs licensed under Creative Commons Attribution 3.0

Thursday, March 13, 2014

Oxytocin: could the 'trust hormone' rebond our troubled world?

Oxytocin: could the 'trust hormone' rebond our troubled world?

Lack of trust is at the root of many of the world's problems, says American neuroeconomist Paul Zak, who claims to have found the brain chemical responsible for empathy. But could oxytocin really help to solve social issues?


Paul Zak
Paul Zak believes oxytocin could help give people more empathy. Photograph: Bryce Duffy
Listening to Paul Zak extol the virtues of oxytocin, the "love hormone", is like hearing a preacher sing the praises of the Promised Land. His idea of a harmonious oxytocin-fuelled society is so seductive you find yourself almost praying it were true. At the same time, you cannot help but wonder if it might be an illusion.

Oxytocin is best known for its use in inducing labour. However, according to Zak, the director of the Centre for Neuroeconomics at Claremont Graduate University, California, it is also the "social glue" that binds families, communities, and societies, and fosters trust between strangers.

To illustrate his point, at a recent appearance at TED Edinburgh, Zak spritzed the backstage staff with oxytocin, prompting a spontaneous outbreak of group hugging. Indeed, such is Zak's faith in the bonding hormone that his licence plate reads "oxytosn". When he texts me to agree a time for our interview, the message reads "From Dr Love's iPhone".

When we finally speak, Zak tells me that "oxytocin is primarily a molecule of social connection. It affects every aspect of social and economic life, from who we choose to make investment decisions on our behalf to how much money we donate to charity. Oxytocin tells us when to trust and when to remain wary, when to give and when to hold back."

But of all Zak's claims, perhaps the one with the most profound social and economic implications, if true, is his assertion that oxytocin is the "essence of empathy". Indeed, Zak believes that his research provides a scientific basis for Scottish Enlightenment thinker Adam Smith's insight that humans are essentially "other-regarding" creatures, imbued at birth with a capacity for "fellow feeling" ("Even the greatest ruffian," Smith wrote in 1759, "is not entirely without it.")
"It's the ultimate moral molecule," says Zak, who describes himself as a "born-again Smithian".

In a summer punctuated by the horrific massacre in Norway and the shocking scenes of destruction and looting in London and other British cities, that claim will strike many as optimistic. But Zak goes further, arguing that many of the social and political issues that currently seem so intractable could be solved if only we could find a way of raising people's basal levels of oxytocin.

As has been demonstrated at Zak's lab in California, one way to achieve this is by getting volunteers to inhale on an oxytocin nasal spray. But trust can also be engendered by less invasive techniques, such as a 15-minute massage or by logging on to social media – practices that Zak has shown also elevate blood oxytocin levels; on a society-wide level, he argues, a similar effect could be achieved by reducing disparities in income, investing in education, and promoting greater freedom and opportunity.

The key, he says, is to kickstart a brain circuit called HOME (human oxytocin-mediated empathy). But can it really be that simple? As the London riots demonstrated, bonding with strangers and trusting the instincts of the herd can lead just as easily to bad behaviour as good. Besides, what does Zak's research tell us that we don't already know: if you're kind to strangers, then your kindness will be reciprocated? Isn't that the message of every great world religion from Christianity to Buddhism?

"We don't need the glamour of neuroscience to tell us that Smith's observations about human nature are correct," argues Raymond Tallis, the author of Aping Mankind: Neuromania, Darwinitis and the Misrepresentation of Humanity. "What is missing in this research is the sense that economic decisions and trust are based on one's interactions at the whole person and the whole community level. The brain is just the middle man."

Oxytocin, an extract from the human posterior pituitary gland, was discovered in 1909 when the British pharmacologist Sir Henry H Dale found it could contract the uterus of a pregnant cat. He named the extract oxytocin, from the Greek for "quick" and "birth". Within two years, doctors were using oxytocin to bring on childbirth contractions. Dale later discovered that oxytocin stimulated the release, or let-down, of mother's milk by contracting the smooth-muscle cells around the mammary glands.

Today, synthetic oxytocin, also called Pitocin or Syntocinon, is often used to induce labour and to help new mothers who have trouble with milk let-down. Oxytocin is also given to women just after birth to prevent postpartum haemorrhage.

By the 1970s, scientists had realised that oxytocin was also a neurotransmitter that acted on the limbic system, the brain's emotional centre. The game-changing insight, however, came from animal studies at the University of Maryland showing that oxytocin played an important role in fostering bonding and monogamous behaviour in prairie voles. In addition, oxytocin has been shown to facilitate nurturing behaviour in mice and rats: when oxytocin was blocked, the rodents stopped caring for their young and displayed signs of "social amnesia".

When Paul Zak, a mathematician and economist by training, stumbled upon this research in the 1990s he had an "aha" moment: the animal studies seemed to describe emerging behaviours of trust and cooperation seen in humans. Like others in his profession, he had grown frustrated with classical economic models that assumed that humans were rational actors who always sought to maximise their individual gains. In his experience, this was not how most people made decisions. He set out to replicate the animal experiments on volunteers engaged in monetary games designed to elicit trust, and then tested their blood for oxytocin. He wondered if the human subjects would show a similar spike in oxytocin?

Shortly after being granted tenure in 2001, Zak told the dean at Claremont that he wouldn't be publishing for a while but instead had acquired a centrifuge and cold freezer for storing blood products. The dean was sceptical. "He told me that I was doing 'vampire economics'," recalls Zak.

There are many variations of the "trust game" but the basic idea is that a person (player one) is given some money and told to send a portion of it to a second person (player two), who has a one-off choice to either accept or reject the proposal. If player two rejects, neither player receives anything. If player two accepts, the money is split according to the proposal. Typically low offers, less than a third of player one's endowment, are rejected as stingy, ensuring that both players get nothing.

To test reciprocity, the game is varied so that both players begin with an equal endowment, £10 say, but this time player one's gift is tripled, and player two is then given the choice of sending some money back to player one (the exchanges are conducted via computer to ensure anonymity). For example, player one offers £3. Player two now has £19 (£10 plus three times £3) and if he were to repay player one's generosity by, for instance, sending him £4 back, player one would leave the game with £11 and both would be better off. In neuroeconomic parlance, the gift from player one is a "trust signal" that prompts player two to reciprocate in kind.

When Zak tested the blood of players who had demonstrated trustworthy behaviour, he found that their oxytocin levels had increased in proportion to the monetary transfer. When he tweaked the experiment by making the transfer amount dependent on the random draw of a ping-pong ball, he found that those who were trusted had oxytocin levels 41% higher than the controls. In other words, it was the signal of trust and not the receipt of money that had prompted the surge of oxytocin.

While this was strong evidence of a correlation, however, it was not proof. Zak re-ran the experiment but this time got half the participants to inhale oxytocin 50 minutes before playing (despite claims that oxytocin induces loving feelings similar to ecstasy, in fact most people notice no change in their affective state). Those who received the oxytocin spray sent back 17% more money compared with the placebo group. Not only that but the number of people who showed maximal trust – sending their entire endowment to a stranger – increased from 21% in the placebo group to 45% in the oxytocin group.

He has achieved these results in repeated tests, including variations of the game to check for cognitive impairment (in one, players are asked to donate earnings to the Red Cross or the Red Crescent Society: oxytocin prompts 48% higher donations but only to the Red Cross, a charity with which north American participants are more familiar and comfortable).

Zak's conclusions are unequivocal. "Trust is chemical," he writes in one paper. "Social norms, one's development history, and even current events affect trust, but these do so by modulating OT release." Or, as he put it to me via his Love phone: "HOME is a positive feedback loop. It literally feels good to do good."
But Zak's proselytism does not end there. In recent papers and a forthcoming book, The Moral Molecule: Vampire Economics and the New Science of Good and Evil, Zak argues that oxytocin holds the key to human morality, policing the "self-other divide" and subtly prodding us towards virtuous behaviour.

For Zak, oxytocin solves the puzzle of why, in practice, people tend to be more trustworthy than the classic economic model predicts. But if we are wired for trust rather than naked self-interest and the oxytocin system is really so powerful, why isn't everyone virtuous all the time, and why did people in the riots trash and burn their own neighbourhoods?

Zak invokes another hormone: testosterone. At times of stress, he argues, we are physiologically in "survival mode", prompting the release of testosterone and its bioactive metabolite, DHT. These stress hormones prevent oxytocin from binding to brain receptors, tipping the balance towards distrust and away from pro-social behaviour. This process, he says, explains "the petty evils normally virtuous people exhibit". In some cases, this bad behaviour may also be exacerbated by genetic and environmental factors.

In his blood-test experiments, for instance, Zak found that 5% of participants did not release oxytocin when trusted. These individuals, he says, "have some of the traits of psychopaths". There is also evidence that the oxytocin receptors in rats' forebrains tend to atrophy when maternal nurturing is "insufficient". Furthermore, he found that women who had been abused in childhood tended not to release oxytocin when prompted by trust signals in the games.
In the case of the riots, Zak believes that most of the looters were probably "neurologically intact". The most likely explanation for the mass criminality was that the line as to what was morally acceptable within social groups in the affected areas shifted, prompting the rioters to identify with "bad" influences. In other words, it was a perfect storm of testosterone and "the wrong kind" of oxytocin.

It is at this point that, according to author Raymond Tallis, the alarm bells should be ringing. "My sense of the riots is that the breakdown in trust didn't result from a sudden, catastrophic oxytocin shortage but relates to such things as education and parental attitudes. In other words, there's nothing that neuroeconomics can tell us that isn't better explained at a sociological level."
Zak argues that by measuring changes in the blood rather than relying on fuzzy images from brain scans, his research is immune to the criticisms usually levelled at neuroscientists. But Tallis questions whether gauging blood oxytocin levels is a substitute for measuring the presence of the hormone in the brain. "I might be similarly disposed to trust my fellow human beings when I've had a couple of pints of beer," he says. "But it doesn't follow that my whole attitude to being more disposed to others is due to my brain alcohol level. Indeed, another couple of pints on another occasion might have a very different effect."

Tallis's fundamental objection, however, is that the game scenarios employed by Zak and other neuroeconomists do not come close to mimicking real-life economic and social interactions. "I don't have anything against oxytocin per se, it's a very fine molecule. It makes breasts secrete milk, it makes uteruses contract, it gives women orgasms. Does it really need another job?"

This is not the first time neuroscience has waxed lyrical about a brain chemical: similar claims were made about dopamine and serotonin. Indeed, in one paper, Zak appears to hedge his bets by showing that these are also involved in the HOME circuit.

However, Zak's faith is unshaken by the sceptics' objections. "Our experiments are as causal as you can get, and the results have been replicated many times," he tells me. "I wish you were nearby so I could put you on some to see how it feels."

It's a tempting offer.

Sunday, March 9, 2014

“Can Empathy Be Taught?”





Can Empathy Be Taught? – Robert Moss

Robert Moss

Big Question

“Can Empathy Be Taught?”


For anyone who has done clinical supervision of graduate students, there is no doubt that there are wide variations in the “natural” abilities of students to utilize warmth, genuineness, and empathy in therapy sessions. The ability of the therapist to convey to a client that there is an accurate perception of his/her emotional state entails both an accurate labeling and description by the therapist. However, those with the more “natural” abilities also display both non-verbal (e.g., facial expressions) and vocal (i.e., tone, inflections, and volume of voice) behaviors that reinforce what is said. So the answer to the big question from my perspective is that “what is said” is much easier to teach than is the “how it is said.”



married-couple-consults-talkin-s

Textbooks and supervisors can provide suggestions on what words to use in situations that can provide the client with the verbal recognition of his/her emotional condition. In relation to the “neurobiology” of empathy, my theory is that it relates to the cortical hemispheres (Moss, 2006, 2007, 2010, 2013; Moss, Hunter, Shah, & Havens, 2012). The left hemisphere for most individuals is dominant for spoken language. Thus, the therapist can learn the verbal components and, in turn, voice this to a client’s left hemisphere relatively easily. Over time a therapist learns more statements and practices these across clients.

If accurate information is given to the client, the verbalized descriptions (processed by the client’s left hemisphere’s “verbal interpreter”) provide an explanation for the non-verbal emotional processing of the client’s right cortex. I (Moss, 2013) have suggested this reduces the conflict (allowing “interhemispheric congruence”) between the two sides with the result being that the client perceives more self-acceptance, internal peace, and may experience an emotional release (e.g., becoming tearful). The self-acceptance involves the left hemisphere’s verbal interpreter recognizing the right hemisphere’s emotion is reasonable (i.e., developing a new schema). The internal peace is a function of decreased inhibitory activity between the right and left frontal lobes. The emotional release is a function of decreased left frontal inhibition on the right frontal lobe which controls the behavioral expression of affective responses.
When the client has a change in affect, this in turn is processed by the therapist’s right hemisphere. It is the right hemisphere processing that allows a therapist to learn experientially to sense when desired affective response is occurring with a client. In turn, the therapist’s right frontal lobe is primarily responsible for the non-verbal and vocal behaviors in terms of “how” therapeutic information is given. In an article I wrote on negative emotional memories, I noted that in relation to learning non-verbal therapy skills:
Clinical experience would be the only means of forming these expressive experiential memories. This leads to the logical conclusion that many experienced, practicing clinicians have more extensive therapy-related experiential right hemisphere receptive and expressive memories than do many academic clinicians. Manualized ESTs cannot teach therapists these right cortical memories since these can only develop as a result of experience with “real world” clients. However, it is possible to use manuals to make therapists verbally aware (i.e., left cortex) of important in-session behaviors that can influence treatment outcome. Additionally, modeled behaviors can be rehearsed by novice therapists with videotaped feedback. Making therapists verbally aware and utilizing experiential training techniques could increase the speed with which these important skills are learned. (Moss, 2007, pp. 218-219)
Few neuroscience professionals will contest the fact that all people learn a “native” verbal language based on the language to which they are exposed during childhood. This appears to occur bilaterally to some degree initially. Yet, the capacity of the left hemisphere to handle the detailed memories tied to spoken language likely leads it to assume final primary control. Similarly, the quick and efficient processing in the right hemisphere allows it to effectively handle the non-detailed analysis and expression of non-verbal emotional components in relationships. Just as we develop a native verbal language of the left hemisphere, we develop a “native emotional language” of the right hemisphere. For those “natural” empathic individuals, the native emotional language they learned as a child involved those non-verbal skills.

The critical period for learning two languages and being truly bilingual is around the age of 8. It is possible to learn a second language later, but it will not ever be perfected. If what I have described is accurate, we learn our native emotional language and this will be with us for the rest of our lives. For those who developed empathic non-verbal skills early, these will remain and only get stronger. Perhaps these individuals have the ability to become “super” therapists, but not even be aware of exactly how they are accomplishing such therapeutic effects. For those who are not “naturals,” they can still get quite efficient in non-verbal therapy skills with sufficient training and experience. However, they will be comparable to someone who developed the use of a second verbal language after the critical period.

I hope this may stimulate some thought for the readers. My articles listed below are available on my contribution page at researchgate.net and academia.edu, as well as at my website www.emotionalrestructuring.com under current papers.

References


Moss, R. A. (2006). Of bits and logic: Cortical columns in learning and memory. The Journal of Mind and Behavior, 27, 215-246.
Moss, R. A. (2007). Negative emotional memories in clinical practice: Theoretical considerations. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 17, 209-224.
Moss, R. A. (2010). Clinical Biopsychology: Could A Grand Theory Actually Exist To Allow True Psychotherapy Integration? Independent Practitioner, 30, 67-71.
Moss, R. A., Hunter, B. P., Shah, D., & Havens, T. (2012). A theory of hemispheric specialization based on cortical columns. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 33, 141-172.
Moss, R. A. (2013). Psychotherapy and the brain: The dimensional systems model and clinical biopsychology. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 34, 63-89.


Bob Moss is a clinical psychologist who works with Bon Secours St. Francis Hospital in Greenville, SC, USA. He is board certified in clinical psychology and neuropsychology. He is a former associate professor in clinical psychology and was in private practice for over 20 years. He has authored a number of professional articles and has presented at a number of local, national, and international professional meetings. His website is www.emotionalrestructuring.com and email is rmoss@emotionalrestructuring.com.

Neuroplasticity, Psychosocial Genomics, and the Biopsychosocial Paradigm in the 21st Century




Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptNIH Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
Health Soc Work. Author manuscript; available in PMC Sep 6, 2010.
Published in final edited form as:
Health Soc Work. Aug 2009; 34(3): 191–199.
PMCID: PMC2933650
NIHMSID: NIHMS217651

Neuroplasticity, Psychosocial Genomics, and the Biopsychosocial Paradigm in the 21st Century


Eric Garland, Ph.D., LCSW
College of Social Work Florida State University Tallahassee, FL 32308
Matthew Owen Howard, Ph.D., Frank Daniels Distinguished Professor of Social Policy

Abstract

The biopsychosocial perspective is a foundation of social work theory and practice. Recent research on neuroplasticity and psychosocial genomics lends compelling support to this perspective by elucidating mechanisms through which psychosocial forces shape neurobiology. Investigations of neuroplasticity demonstrate that the adult brain can continue to form novel neural connections and grow new neurons in response to learning or training even into old age. These findings are complemented by the contributions of psychosocial genomics, a field of scientific inquiry that explores the modulating effects of experience on gene expression. Findings from these new sciences provide external validation for the biopsychosocial perspective and offer important insights into the manifold means by which socioenvironmental experiences influence neurobiological structure and function across the life course.

Keywords: biopsychosocial, neuroplasticity, psychosocial genomics, gene-environment interaction

Introduction


Social work professionals in the 21st Century have adopted the biopsychosocial paradigm. This paradigm, first articulated by the physician, George Engel, holds that humans are dynamic systems whose functioning depends on the holistic integration of biological, psychological, and social factors (Engel, 1977); indeed, according to the biopsychosocial model, these factors are fundamentally interrelated and interdependent. Although Engel rejected the reductionism of the dominant biomedical model of his era, which assumed that molecular biological processes (e.g., genes and biochemistry) immutably dictated physiology and behavior, a simple-minded biological determinism nonetheless took root and became widely, if uncritically, accepted. At its inception, there was scant evidence to support Engel's biopsychosocial perspective; however, scientific discoveries of the past decade have provided important new findings validating and elaborating the biopsychosocial paradigm.

Over the past decade, two fields of empirical investigation, neuroplasticity and psychosocial genomics, have offered important findings that may lead to a paradigm shift in our conceptions of psyche and soma and the modes of their interrelationships. These two fields mutually inform one another, depicting interpenetrating biopsychosocial relationships on different scales: neuroplasticity research describes how neurons within the brain proliferate and grow new connections across the lifespan, whereas psychosocial genomics describes the processes by which psychological and social experiences activate or deactivate genes, thereby driving the development of new neural pathways. The interplay of these sciences reflects a vision of humans as inherently resilient; psychosocial factors appear to stimulate gene expression within neurons resulting in alterations to the structure and function of the brain. Discoveries from both fields reveal that experience and learning can contribute to positive change, even at the neurobiological and structural levels.

Social work academicians have embraced the biopsychosocial perspective; yet, many are perhaps not fully aware of recent developments in genomic and neurobiological research with implications for social work and the biopsychosocial perspective. This research provides insights into the very substrates of biopsychosocial change. Thus, we review recent neuroplasticity and psychosocial genomics research and its implications for current understanding and application of the biopsychosocial perspective.

Neuroplasticity

Basic neurotransmission


The human brain is a complex, self-organizing, biological system, consisting of trillions of interconnected nerve cells called neurons. The operation of neurons results in two distinct forms of information processing: signaling and integration. Each neuron propagates signals via action potentials, electrochemical currents that travel the length of its axon. This current leads to the release of neurotransmitters which traverse synapses, the gaps between neurons. These chemical messages are received via specialized receptor cells at the ends of numerous, tree-like branches of the receiving neuron, called dendrites. The stimulation of dendritic receptors by neurotransmitters leads to integration, whereby large amounts of information from many neurons is summed up before reaching a threshold to fire the action potential down the next axon. In this manner, perceptual information from the external environment and the internal milieu of the body is transmitted and processed in the brain, leading to cognition, emotion, and behavior, the essence of human experience.

Origins of neuroplasticity research


The brains of infants and children are known to be plastic, undergoing spurts of neuronal development in response to stimulus exposure during critical periods (Mundkur, 2005). This development consists of the genesis of neurons, increased connectivity between extant neurons, and the routing of new synaptic connections between previously unrelated neurons. However, before 1998, it was widely accepted that neuronal connections in the adult brain were immutable; the neurons that populated a given brain area were thought to be fixed in accordance with whatever form and function the genetic code prescribed for that region (Begley, 2006). In addition, the conventional wisdom at the time--that no new neurons could be generated after injury or insult to the brain—was held with conviction on the part of leading neuroscientists.

However, upon discovery of the growth of new neural tissue, or neurogenesis, in the adult human hippocampus, a brain region responsible for memory (Eriksson et al., 1998), the dogma of the “hardwired brain” was formally repudiated. This finding complemented earlier evidence from primate studies demonstrating that novel sensory experience and learning new behaviors triggers neuronal growth in the somatosensory and motor cortices, areas of the brain subserving tactile perception and limb movement (Jenkins, Merzenich, Ochs, Allard, & Guic-Robles, 1990; Nudo, Milliken, Jenkins, & Merzenich, 1996). Subsequent to the discovery of neurogenesis in the adult human brain, neuroscience has pursued this line of investigation with vigor, aided by advances in brain imaging techniques such as magnetic-resonance imaging.

Neuroplasticity research findings


The growth of neurons has been documented in the brains of adults exposed to a variety of experiences. For instance, violinists evidence neural growth in the portion of their somatosensory cortex devoted to their fingering hand through hours of musical practice (Elbert, Pantev, Wienbruch, Rockstroh, & Taub, 1995), as do persons engaged in the practice of juggling (Draganski et al., 2004). In addition to such physical training, mental practice may promote neuroplasticity: neurogenesis can occur in the motor cortex just by imagining playing the piano (Pascual-Leone, Amedi, Fregni, & Merabet, 2005). Similarly, taxicab drivers develop the areas of their brains involved in spatial relationships by memorizing the labyrinthine streets and avenues of the cities in which they work (Maguire et al., 2000). While the underlying mechanisms are different, neuroplasticity research suggests that challenging learning experiences can lead to the development of brain tissue analogous to the way physical exercise can lead to the development of muscle tissue.

One area of research that has found significant evidence of mental training leading to neuroplastic modifications in brain activity focuses on the study of meditation. Meditation, while greatly varying in technique and purpose across the diverse spiritual and cultural traditions where it is employed, may be generally defined as the intentional practice whereby one grasps “the handle of cognition” to cultivate a competent use of his or her own mental capacities, gaining agency over thought and emotion (Depraz, Varela, & Vermersch, 2003). Such intentional mental training has been shown to induce functional neurobiological changes.

A study by Lutz and colleagues found marked alterations in the synchronization of neurons as an effect of long-term training in Buddhist loving-kindness meditation, a practice which is thought by some practitioners to promote a state of unconditional compassion and benevolence (Lutz, Greischar, Rawlings, Ricard, & Davidson, 2004). Neural synchrony of the type observed in this study may be indicative of coherent and integrated psychological functioning (Williams et al., 2005). The synchronization of brain activity found in some of the practitioners sampled, whose experience ranged between 10,000 and 50,000 hours spent in meditation, was higher than any previously reported in the literature. Such increased neural synchrony was observed not only during the meditative state, but also when the practitioners were not meditating, suggesting that long-term mental practice can induce lasting, trait-level changes possibly mediated by structural modifications to the brain (Begley, 2006).

Other research has documented changes in neurobiological function as a result of mindfulness meditation, the practice of cultivating a present-centered, metacognitive awareness, “a naturalistic state wherein consciousness transcends its content to rest upon the dynamics of its own processes” (Garland, 2007). A recent study by Slagter et al. (2007) compared attentional performance of a group of experienced meditators participating in a 3-month mindfulness meditation retreat to that of a novice control group who received a 1-hour meditation class and were asked to meditate 20 minutes daily for one week. Relative to controls, experienced meditators evidenced significant improvements in attentional performance that correlated with alterations in brain activity. This cognitive enhancement was maintained 3 months after formal meditation practice, providing suggestive evidence that mental training can stimulate neuroplastic changes in the adult human brain (Slagter et al., 2007).

While the work of Slagter et al. and Lutz et al. provide tentative support for meditation-induced neuroplasticity, neither study examined structural brain changes per se. However, two structural MRI investigations comparing the brains of experienced meditators to control subjects matched in sex, age, race, and years of education found that years of meditation experience correlated with increased cortical thickness in brain areas where visceral attention (e.g. right anterior insula) and self-awareness (e.g. left superior temporal gyrus) have been localized (Holzel et al., 2008; Lazar et al., 2005). These empirical investigations of meditation suggest that mental training may stimulate structural alterations reflective of neuroplasticity.

Clinical implications of neuroplasticity research


The finding that experience and training can lead to the development of new neural connections has key implications. For example, persons suffering from what was once thought to be permanent brain injury can heal through rehabilitation designed to stimulate the damaged area, such as in the case of stroke (Taub et al., 2006). However, although largely speculative, it is possible that neuroplasticity may undergird not only rehabilitation of physical illness but that of select psychological disorders as well, mediating natural recovery from mental illness in some cases as well as improvements related to psychosocial interventions. At present, it has been demonstrated that psychotherapy can induce functional changes in brain activation. For example, a brain imaging study found that persons with obsessive-compulsive disorder who were treated with a mindfulness-oriented form of cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) exhibited functional changes in the orbital frontal cortex and striatum, two brain structures found to be overactive in OCD (Schwartz & Begley, 2002). Other studies have demonstrated psychotherapy-related alterations in brain circuits involved in depression (e.g. Goldapple et al., 2004; Martin, Martin, Rai, Richardson, & Royall, 2001). CBT has also been associated with changes in frontal and temporal brain regions of persons suffering from panic disorder (Prasko et al., 2004). Such intervention-related changes in both psychosocial function and neural activity may correlate with neuroplastic alterations to the brain; critically, a combined functional and structural magnetic resonance imaging study of practice-induced increases in gray matter found that increased task-specific brain activation led to the remodeling of one of the same neural structures (i.e. dorsolateral occipital cortex) that was activated by the practice and learning of the task (Ilg et al., 2008).

Neuroplasticity research of psychosocial interventions has just begun. A recent longitudinal study of cognitive-behavioral therapy for women with chronic fatigue syndrome found increases in gray matter of the lateral prefrontal cortex after 16 sessions of CBT (de Lange et al., 2008). Increases in gray matter volume correlated with enhanced cognitive processing speed, suggesting that the neuroplasticity evoked by psychotherapy played a causal role in rehabilitation of cognitive performance after cerebral atrophy resulting from chronic fatigue.
Indeed, neuroplasticity may be the biological mechanism through which psychosocial interventions exert at least some of their therapeutic effects. During psychotherapy, when the client recalls negative or painful life experiences, the clinician may assist in reframing the context so that the experience gains new meaning (de Shazer, 1988). For instance, in treating persons who have experienced traumas such as rape, therapy may help clients to envision themselves as a survivor rather than as a victim. Such reframing or reappraisal may be a critical component of successful biopsychosocial outcomes (Folkman, 1997; Penley, Tomaka, & Wiebe, 2002). Some theorists hypothesize that the process of recalling, reconstructing, and reframing memories of past trauma during psychotherapy is mediated by the reorganization and genesis of neurons (Centonze, Siracusano, Calabresi, & Bernardi, 2005; E.L. Rossi, 2005). This hypothesis is founded on evidence that the formation of new long-term memories results from neuroplastic changes in the brain structure known as the hippocampus. Hippocampal changes appear within hours of significant learning experiences (McGaugh, 2000), such as those that can occur during psychotherapy.

Neuroplasticity is mediated at the cellular level through activity-dependent gene expression, the mechanism by which neurons secrete growth factors leading to the “activation of gene transcription in the nucleus that support[s] synaptic connections…Thus, with every new experience, the brain slightly rewires its physical structure and this rewiring is mediated through the signaling cascade” (Mundkur, 2005). Hence, in order to understand neuroplasticity, we must consider the domain of psychosocial genomics.

Psychosocial Genomics

Basic epigenetics


In the 21st century, there is broad agreement that the genome is the basis of human life and a precondition for psychosocial experience. Nevertheless, the question of the respective roles of nature and nurture in human experience and the manner of their interaction in select contexts remains contentious, despite the more than half-century that has transpired since Watson and Crick (1953) identified DNA as the building block of biological processes.

The DNA code of the human genome does not determine protein synthesis in a one-toone fashion; instead, genes are subject to epigenetic processes (i.e. modifications that do not occur due to changes in the basic genetic sequence of amino acids but that instead result from biological and environmental influences on the expression of genes as proteins) (Eisenberg, 2004). During gene expression, the genetic code serves as a “blueprint” that guides the construction of proteins from amino acids. However, this construction process is modulated by signals from the internal and external environments, which steer and modify the manner in which basic organic molecules are organized into anatomy and physiology. Although genes prescribe protein synthesis, there is substantial variability in the manner in which they are expressed.

A single genotype, the genetic blueprint of an organism, can be expressed in a multiplicity of distinct physiological and behavioral forms, known as phenotypes. This is evident in Eisenberg's (2004) example of phenylketonuria, a disorder that when untreated may lead to severe mental retardation, psychosis, and seizures. If children with this genetic abnormality are kept on a postnatal diet low in the amino acid phenyalanine, they do not develop these disorders. Hence, although the genotype for phenylketonuria does not change, its phenotypic expression is modified by the environment (i.e., nutrition) to which the individual has been exposed. The mechanisms by which such different phenotypes are expressed are just beginning to be understood, but appear to involve the regulatory effect of internal and external environmental signals on stress hormones, which in turn modify gene transcription processes (Kandel, 1998; E. L. Rossi, 2004).

Learning and other psychosocial experiences may modulate gene expression


In addition to physical environmental forces, learning experiences in the social environment can alter gene expression (McCutcheon, 2006). The bi-directional relationship of nature and nurture, genes and environment, was first demonstrated in a series of path-breaking studies of maternal care in rats (Francis, Champagne, Liu, & Meaney, 1999; Liu et al., 1997). In these studies, an inverse relationship was found between the number of stress hormone receptors in a rat's hippocampus and its tendency to exhibit stress reactions. The number of these receptors is dictated by the genotype of the rat. Highly stress-reactive rats give low levels of maternal care to their offspring, who, in turn, exhibit high stress reactivity and later provide low levels of maternal care to their offspring. However, these studies revealed that hormonal and behavioral stress reactions of rat pups as well as the number of their stress hormone receptors are modulated by the licking, grooming, and nursing behaviors of their mothers. Even if a rat were born with a genotype coding for fewer stress hormone receptors, if it was reared by an adoptive mother providing high levels of maternal care, the rat's genes produced more stress receptors, making it calmer, less reactive to stressors, and more apt to provide maternal care to its offspring. These findings offer some evidence that social behavior may be inherited and transduced via gene expression into neuroplastic alterations in brain structure, leading to psychobiological learning and change.

The notion that social experience can lead to changes in gene expression was voiced most prominently by Nobel laureate, Eric Kandel, who regarded this observation as the core component of a new paradigm for psychiatry (1998). Kandel summarized the current state of biological thinking with regard to the relation between social experiences and neurobiology, observing that:
The regulation of gene expression by social factors makes all bodily functions, including all functions of the brain, susceptible to social influences. These social influences will be biologically incorporated in the altered expressions of specific genes in specific nerve cells of specific regions of the brain. These socially influenced alterations are transmitted culturally (Kandel, 1998, p. 461).
This powerful claim, while supported by over a decade of rigorous research, has rarely been directly tested. However, advances in psychoendoneuroimmunology, the study of how mental processes affect the immune system, have clearly shown the effects of psychological and social factors on human physiological functions that indirectly involve the genetic replication of cells (Ray, 2004). Such alterations of biological function may be mediated through experience-dependent gene expression, the process whereby social-environmental signals turn genes “on” and “off,” leading to alterations in protein synthesis which ultimately result in physiological changes (Pinaud, 2004).

Psychosocial genomic hypotheses


Although our genes provide a basic outline for development, environmental influences such as social experiences shape gene expression and ultimately make us unique individuals. This interaction is the essence of what Rossi (2002) has termed “psychosocial genomics,” the interdisciplinary study of the processes by which gene expression is modulated by psychological, social, and cultural experiences. Practitioners might profit from knowing more about this new science, for according to Kandel:
Insofar as psychotherapy or counseling is effective and produces long-term changes in behavior, it presumably does so though learning, through producing changes in gene expression that alter the strength of synaptic connections and structural changes that alter the anatomical pattern of nerve cells of the brain (Kandel, 1998, p. 460)
Thus, it is conceivable that psychosocial interventions, the tools of social work practice, may produce alterations in gene expression leading, in some cases, to measurable neurobiological changes. Since can stress affect neurogenesis through alterations in gene expression and transcription (Glaser et al., 1990; Warner-Schmidt & Duman, 2006), ultimately leading to dysregulation of affect (Post, 1992), psychosocial interventions designed to reduce distress and improve mood may affect brain structure and function through this pathway. Muenke (2008) has recently suggested that the therapeutic effects of stress-reduction techniques might be mediated by changes in gene expression. In line with this hypothesis, a recent study of a meditative breathing practice found increased gene expression of the immune factors glutathione S-transferase, Cox-2, and HSP-70 in practitioners relative to controls (Sharmaa et al., 2008). While this study supports the psychosocial genomic hypothesis, its cross-sectional design does not allow for confident inferences vis-à-vis causality. However, in light of this potential shortcoming, a longitudinal study examined gene expression before and after exposure to eight weeks of meditation training (Dusek et al., 2008), and found alterations in the expression of 1561 genes after the intervention. Among these changes were increases in the expression of genes associated with the stress response, suggesting that learning to engage the relaxation response through meditation may attenuate the deleterious impact of stress on cellular processes.

Although controlled psychosocial genomic research is uncommon, there are a growing number of psychosocial intervention studies that do measure physiological outcomes such as blood levels of cortisol or immune factors. For instance, stress reduction interventions have been shown to increase numbers of immune cells and decrease numbers of cells associated with allergic reactivity (Castes et al., 1999), and improve antibody response to the flu vaccine (Davidson et al., 2003). Intervention-related changes in such biological markers may serve as indirect measures of alterations in gene expression.

The new scientific paradigm outlined above provides a perspective on how the biopsychosocial constitutions of practitioners and clients might interact in the act of therapy:
When a therapist speaks to a patient and the patient listens, the therapist is not only making eye contact and voice contact, but the action of neuronal machinery in the therapist's brain is having an indirect, and, one hopes, long-lasting effect on the neuronal machinery in the patient's brain; and quite likely, vice versa. Insofar as our words produce changes in our patient's mind, it is likely that these psychotherapeutic interventions produce changes in the patient's brain. From this perspective, the biological and sociopsychological approaches are joined. (Kandel, 1998, p. 466)
The union of neuroplasticity and psychosocial genomics research represents a synthesis of the social and biological sciences that is non-reductive: it does not dismiss human experience as the product of a neural machine, pre-determined by its genetic blueprint. Instead, it is integrative, inclusive, and holistic; this unitary approach reveals the power of thought and emotion, society and culture to affect not only our phenomenological experience but our very neurobiological structure and function. In sharp contrast to genetic determinism, this new paradigm envisions individuals as having the innate potential for agency over the tripartite dimensionality of their biopsychosocial selves.

Implications for Social Work

The social work profession's historical emphasis on the social environment as the context for individual well-being is supported by research over the past decade. Neuroplasticity and psychosocial genomic research indicate that socioenvironmental forces have the potency to alter human well-being through their effects on neurobiology. Social experience may be transduced through the activation of neurons, leading to modifications in the phenotypic expression of genes and eventuating in structural changes to the brain. While genes and neurobiology may be the substrates of vulnerability to environmental stressors, they are also, in all likelihood, the substrates of resilience (D Cicchetti, 2003; D. Cicchetti & Blender, 2006).

The sciences of neuroplasticity and psychosocial genomics may provide new empirical bases for social work interventions. Biological measures of change can and should be used to enhance the evaluation of social intervention research. Given the current funding climate and priorities of the National Institutes of Health, research programs designed to evaluate social work practice might be more likely to obtain grant support if interventions studied were evaluated with physiological outcome measures including those assessing gene expression and neuroplasticity. In time, a given practice may be deemed “evidence-based” when, among other criteria, it is shown to result in plastic brain changes or altered gene expression associated with improved biopsychosocial functioning.

Currently, there is a paucity of empirical support for this new paradigm in studies with humans. An abundance of research on higher mammals indicates that experience can trigger gene expression leading to neuroplasticity. As referenced earlier in this paper, several studies on humans indicate that learning and training led to neurogenesis and the reorganization of neural networks. Despite developments in these lines of research, science has only begun to examine the effects of psychosocial interventions on brain structure and function. More research must be conducted in this emerging field, and the social work profession, with its expertise in addressing social problems and enhancing human well-being, can make a vital contribution to this endeavor.

Brain imaging and gene assays may be utilized to detect the neuroplastic and genomic effects of psychosocial interventions. Technologies such as magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and positron emission tomography (PET) are capable of assessing the neurophysiological changes associated with psychosocial interventions (Kumari, 2006). Reductions in psychiatric symptoms may be reflected in the alterations in brain metabolism and structure revealed by these imaging technologies. DNA microarray technologies, which can evaluate messenger RNA production in cells and thereby determine which genes are activated (Mirnics, Middleton, Lewis, & Levitt, 2001; Raychaudhuri, Sutphin, Chang, & Altman, 2001), have been used to assess alterations in gene expression related to post-traumatic stress disorder (Segman et al., 2005), social aggression (Berton et al., 2006), and depression (Evans et al., 2004). DNA microarrays may become more widely used to measure biological effects of psychosocial interventions in the not-too-distant future (E. L. Rossi, 2005).

Nevertheless, the funding and specialized training necessary to perform brain imaging and DNA microarrays decreases the likelihood that social work researchers working in isolation could leverage these technologies for biopsychosocial research. Consequently, future psychosocial intervention research could involve interdisciplinary teams of social workers, neuroscientists, and molecular biologists, where data from the biological sciences could be complemented by the insights of social work research. Alternatively, other more accessible biological markers, such as stress hormone levels in saliva, could be measured as a proxy for physiological change induced by psychosocial interventions. For example, salivary cortisol assays are a relatively inexpensive form of assessment that can be done by many university laboratories. Social work investigators could add this measure to their intervention research protocols.

Whether the impact of psychosocial interventions can be traced at the neuronal, genomic, or grosser levels of physiological response, biological markers will only be meaningful as a complement to self-report and collateral measures of change. Indeed, Engel's biopsychosocial paradigm is rooted in the philosophical principle of complementarity (Freedman, 1995); instead of the “either/or” mentality of dualistic reductionism, biopsychosocial research should embrace a “both/and” logic, where reports of subjective experience garnered through validated instruments and qualitative interviews are correlated with biological and behavioral data. Such research can add value to Social Work as a primary mental health and allied-health profession and lead to the implementation of interventions with demonstrable physiological, psychological, and behavioral benefits.

Conclusion

Over the past decade neuroplasticity research has enriched the biopsychosocial perspective by demonstrating that psychosocial experiences not only influence neurobiological processes but may actually change the structure of the adult brain. These structural changes consist of increased arborization of neurons, enhanced synaptic connectivity, and even the genesis of new neural tissue. Although neuroplasticity research is in its infancy, recent findings suggest that the effects of psychosocial experiences such learning and mental training on cognitive, emotional, and behavioral functions may be mediated by alterations to the architecture of the brain.

In turn, experience-dependent modifications to neural tissue may be driven by epigenetic processes (i.e., changes in gene expression produced by environmental determinants). The human environment is constantly conditioned by social experiences, which, when transduced by the nervous system into electrochemical signals, may modulate protein synthesis in the nuclei of nerve cells, ultimately leading to changes in the replication and growth of neurons. Social experience can change gene expression, leading to the restructuring of the brain through neuroplasticity. While tentative at present, empirical investigations of the psychosocial genomic hypothesis will likely proliferate over the next decade.

These new biopsychosocial sciences are consistent with a view of human beings as holistic, recursive systems structurally coupled with their environments in a process of mutual change (Maturana & Varela, 1987). Intentionality and volition can generate changes in the structure of the brain, the very organ assumed to produce such mental phenomena (Schwartz & Begley, 2002). With this finding it is evident that human experience is not driven solely from the bottom-up by neurobiology and genetics. Instead, there is growing evidence that psychosocial experience can exert a macrodeterministic, top-down force upon our biology. In the philosophy of emergent interactionism, Roger Sperry, Nobel laureate neuroscientist, described macrodeterminism as a higher-order, molar level of organization that determines and conditions the activity of lower-order, nested sub-components (Sperry, 1987). Hence, human beings, who are at one level assemblies of organ systems comprised of aggregates of cells, in turn composed of organic molecules made up of sub-atomic particles, are not merely the summation of these physical elements. Instead, the consciousness that emerges from the interaction of these components can act back upon its physical substrate. Thought, emotion, and action trigger neural activity, which can lead to a re-organization of the brain, shaping future psychosocial experience. From this perspective, we are not the passive products of neurophysiology and heredity; rather, through our behavior in the social environment, we become active agents in the construction of our own neurobiology, and ultimately, our own lives.

This new paradigm may reveal the empirical foundation of that most central of social work principles, the idea that people have the power to transcend and transform their limitations into opportunities for growth and well-being.

Acknowledgments

ELG was supported by Grant Number T32AT003378 from the National Center for Complementary and Alternative Medicine.

References

  • Begley S. Train your mind, change your brain: How a new science reveals our extraordinary potential to transform ourselves. Ballantine; New York: 2006.
  • Berton O, McClung CA, Dileone RJ, Krishnan V, Renthal W, Russo SJ, et al. Essential role of BDNF in the mesolimbic dopamine pathway in social defeat stress. Science. 2006;311(5762):864–868. [PubMed]
  • Castes M, Hagel I, Palenque M, Canelones P, Corao A, Lynch NR. Immunological changes associated with clinical improvement of asthmatic children subjected to psychosocial intervention. Brain Behav Immun. 1999;13(1):1–13. [PubMed]
  • Centonze D, Siracusano A, Calabresi P, Bernardi G. Removing pathogenic memories: a neurobiology of psychotherapy. Mol Neurobiol. 2005;32(2):123–132. [PubMed]
  • Cicchetti D. Foreward. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 2003.
  • Cicchetti D, Blender JA. A multiple-levels-of-analysis perspective on resilience: implications for the developing brain, neural plasticity, and preventive interventions. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2006;1094:248–258. [PubMed]
  • Davidson RJ, Kabat-Zinn J, Schumacher J, Rosenkranz M, Muller D, Santorelli SF, et al. Alterations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation. Psychosom Med. 2003;65(4):564–570. [PubMed]
  • de Lange FP, Koers A, Kalkman JS, Bleijenberg G, Hagoort P, van der Meer JW, et al. Increase in prefrontal cortical volume following cognitive behavioural therapy in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome. Brain. 2008;131(Pt 8):2172–2180. [PubMed]
  • de Shazer S. Clues: Investigating solutions in brief therapy. W.W. Norton & Company; New York: 1988.
  • Depraz N, Varela F, Vermersch P. On becoming aware. John Benjamins North America; Philadelphia: 2003.
  • Draganski B, Gaser C, Busch V, Schuierer G, Bogdahn U, May A. Neuroplasticity: changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature. 2004;427(6972):311–312. [PubMed]
  • Dusek JA, Otu HH, Wohlhueter AL, Bhasin M, Zerbini LF, Joseph MG, et al. Genomic counter-stress changes induced by the relaxation response. PLoS ONE. 2008;3(7):e2576. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Eisenberg L. Social psychiatry and the human genome: contextualising heritability. Br J Psychiatry. 2004;184:101–103. [PubMed]
  • Elbert T, Pantev C, Wienbruch C, Rockstroh B, Taub E. Increased cortical representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players. Science. 1995;270(5234):305–307. [PubMed]
  • Engel GL. The need for a new medical model: A challenge for biomedicine. Science. 1977;196(4286):129–136. [PubMed]
  • Eriksson PS, Perfilieva E, Bjork-Eriksson T, Alborn AM, Nordborg C, Peterson DA, et al. Neurogenesis in the adult human hippocampus. Nat Med. 1998;4(11):1313–1317. [PubMed]
  • Evans SJ, Choudary PV, Neal CR, Li JZ, Vawter MP, Tomita H, et al. Dysregulation of the fibroblast growth factor system in major depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101(43):15506–15511. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Folkman S. Positive psychological states and coping with severe stress. Soc Sci Med. 1997;45(8):1207–1221. [PubMed]
  • Francis DD, Champagne FA, Liu D, Meaney MJ. Maternal care, gene expression, and the development of individual differences in stress reactivity. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999;896:66–84. [PubMed]
  • Freedman AM. The biopsychosocial paradigm and the future of psychiatry. Compr Psychiatry. 1995;36(6):397–406. [PubMed]
  • Garland EL. The meaning of mindfulness: A second-order cybernetics of stress, metacognition, and coping. Complementary Health Practice Review. 2007;12(1):15–30.
  • Glaser R, Kennedy S, Lafuse WP, Bonneau RH, Speicher C, Hillhouse J, et al. Psychological stress-induced modulation of interleukin 2 receptor gene expression and interleukin 2 production in peripheral blood leukocytes. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1990;47(8):707–712. [PubMed]
  • Goldapple K, Segal Z, Garson C, Lau M, Bieling P, Kennedy S, et al. Modulation of cortical-limbic pathways in major depression: Treatment-specific effects of cognitive behavior therapy. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61(1):34–41. [PubMed]
  • Holzel BK, Ott U, Gard T, Hempel H, Weygandt M, Morgen K, et al. Investigation of mindfulness meditation practitioners with voxel-based morphometry. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2008;3(1):55–61. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Ilg R, Wohlschlager AM, Gaser C, Liebau Y, Dauner R, Woller A, et al. Gray matter increase induced by practice correlates with task-specific activation: a combined functional and morphometric magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci. 2008;28(16):4210–4215. [PubMed]
  • Jenkins WM, Merzenich MM, Ochs MT, Allard T, Guic-Robles E. Functional reorganization of primary somatosensory cortex in adult owl monkeys after behaviorally controlled tactile stimulation. J Neurophysiol. 1990;63(1):82–104. [PubMed]
  • Kandel ER. A new intellectual framework for psychiatry. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155(4):457–469. [PubMed]
  • Kumari V. Do psychotherapies produce neurobiological effects? Acta Neuropsychiatrica. 2006;18:61–70.
  • Lazar SW, Kerr CE, Wasserman RH, Gray JR, Greve DN, Treadway MT, et al. Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness. Neuroreport. 2005;16(17):1893–1897. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Liu D, Diorio J, Tannenbaum B, Caldji C, Francis D, Freedman A, et al. Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. Science. 1997;277(5332):1659–1662. [PubMed]
  • Lutz A, Greischar LL, Rawlings NB, Ricard M, Davidson RJ. Long-term meditators self-induce high-amplitude gamma synchrony during mental practice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101(46):16369–16373. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Maguire EA, Gadian DG, Johnsrude IS, Good CD, Ashburner J, Frackowiak RS, et al. Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2000;97(8):4398–4403. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Martin S, Martin E, Rai S, Richardson M, Royall R. Brain blood flow changes in depressed patients treated with interpersonal psychotherapy or venlafaxine hydrochloride: Preliminary findings. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58:641–648. [PubMed]
  • Maturana H, Varela F. The tree of knowledge: The biological roots of human understanding. Shambala; Boston: 1987.
  • McCutcheon V. Toward an integration of social and biological research. Social Service Review. 2006;80(1):159–178. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • McGaugh JL. Memory--a century of consolidation. Science. 2000;287(5451):248–251. [PubMed]
  • Mirnics K, Middleton FA, Lewis DA, Levitt P. The human genome: gene expression profiling and schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(9):1384. [PubMed]
  • Muenke M. A paradigm shift in genetics: How hypnosis influences our genes. 59th Annual Society for Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis.2008.
  • Mundkur N. Neuroplasticity in children. Indian J Pediatr. 2005;72(10):855–857. [PubMed]
  • Nudo RJ, Milliken GW, Jenkins WM, Merzenich MM. Use-dependent alterations of movement representations in primary motor cortex of adult squirrel monkeys. J Neurosci. 1996;16(2):785–807. [PubMed]
  • Pascual-Leone A, Amedi A, Fregni F, Merabet LB. The plastic human brain cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2005;28:377–401. [PubMed]
  • Penley JA, Tomaka J, Wiebe JS. The association of coping to physical and psychological health outcomes: a meta-analytic review. J Behav Med. 2002;25(6):551–603. [PubMed]
  • Pinaud R. Experience-dependent immediate early gene expression in the adult central nervous system: Evidence from enriched-environment studies. International Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;114:321–333. [PubMed]
  • Post RM. Transduction of psychosocial stress into the neurobiology of recurrent affective disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 1992;149(8):999–1010. [PubMed]
  • Prasko J, Horacek J, Zalesky R, Kopecek M, Novak T, Paskova B, et al. The change of regional brain metabolism (18FDG PET) in panic disorder during the treatment with cognitive behavioral therapy or antidepressants. Neuro Endocrinology Letters. 2004;25(5):340–348. [PubMed]
  • Ray O. The revolutionary health science of psychoendoneuroimmunology: a new paradigm for understanding health and treating illness. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004;1032:35–51. [PubMed]
  • Raychaudhuri S, Sutphin PD, Chang JT, Altman RB. Basic microarray analysis: grouping and feature reduction. Trends Biotechnol. 2001;19(5):189–193. [PubMed]
  • Rossi EL. Psychosocial genomics: gene expression, neurogenesis, and human experience in mind-body medicine. Advances in Mind Body Medicine. 2002;18(2):22–30. [PubMed]
  • Rossi EL. Stress-induced alternative gene splicing in mind-body medicine. Adv Mind Body Med. 2004;20(2):12–19. [PubMed]
  • Rossi EL. The ideodynamic action hypothesis of therapeutic suggestion: Creative replay in the psychosocial genomics of therapeutic hypnosis. European Journal of Clinical Hypnosis. 2005;6(2):2–12.
  • Rossi EL. Prospects for exploring the molecular-genomic foundations of therapeutic hypnosis with DNA microarrays. Am J Clin Hypn. 2005;48(2–3):165–182. [PubMed]
  • Schwartz JM, Begley S. The mind and the brain: Neuroplasticity and the power of mental force. Harper Collins; New York: 2002.
  • Segman RH, Shefi N, Goltser-Dubner T, Friedman N, Kaminski N, Shalev AY. Peripheral blood mononuclear cell gene expression profiles identify emergent post-traumatic stress disorder among trauma survivors. Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10(5):500–513. 425. [PubMed]
  • Sharmaa H, Dattaa P, Singha A, Sena S, Bhardwajb N, Kochupillaib V, et al. Gene expression profiling in practitioners of Sudarshan Kriya. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2008;64:213–218. [PubMed]
  • Slagter HA, Lutz A, Greischar LL, Francis AD, Nieuwenhuis S, Davis JM, et al. Mental training affects distribution of limited brain resources. PLoS Biol. 2007;5(6):e138. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Sperry R. Structure and significance of the consciousness revolution. Journal of Mind and Behavior. 1987;8(1)
  • Taub E, Uswatte G, King DK, Morris D, Crago JE, Chatterjee A. A placebo-controlled trial of constraint-induced movement therapy for upper extremity after stroke. Stroke. 2006;37(4):1045–1049. [PubMed]
  • Warner-Schmidt JL, Duman RS. Hippocampal neurogenesis: opposing effects of stress and antidepressant treatment. Hippocampus. 2006;16(3):239–249. [PubMed]
  • Watson JD, Crick FH. Genetical implications of the structure of deoxyribonucleic acid. Nature. 1953;171(4361):964–967. [PubMed]
  • Williams LM, Grieve SM, Whitford TJ, Clark CR, Gur RC, Goldberg E, et al. Neural synchrony and gray matter variation in human males and females: integration of 40 Hz gamma synchrony and MRI measures. J Integr Neurosci. 2005;4(1):77–93. [PubMed]